What kind of scientist are you?
29 March, 2011 | Morgan Giddings |
|
|
The student was at the board, hands covered in chalk, mixed with a bit of sweat. He was in the middle of his oral prelim exam, and being grilled about his hypothesis. How would he design his experiments to test it? Did he have the proper controls in place? Would his measurements be within the limits of accuracy?
He struggled to get the answers to the questions out, with a gripping fear that his experimental design would be inadequate, and that the committee would shoot it all down, making him repeat this exercise over again next year.
Yet in all the questioning posed by the serious professors, and in all the fear that the student was experiencing, there was an elephant in the room that nobody discussed: was the hypothesis good enough to begin with? Were the questions really worth asking? If they weren’t, how would he improve them?
Some students had flunked out at the written prelim stage due to having poorly constructed hypotheses and questions. Sometimes it was difficult to separate bad writing from inadequate ideas. But in every case, the students were sent back to do it all over again, without a lot of guidance on a key point: how do you come up with really good questions?
If you think this is just a minor issue, then consider Albert Einstein for a second.
Was he famous for his experimental design acumen? No. In fact, he was so poor at certain left-brained activities that he flunked High School Algebra.
Rather, he was famous for right-brained activities, like asking great questions and having brilliant new insights. He synthesized disparate observations and facts into a new whole, and translated that into elegant mathematical language.
Since Einstein’s time, many people have designed and implemented experiments that test various aspects of Einstein’s theories. That is important work. But is it as important as the original act of creation of the theories? Einstein’s name will always be associated with “relativity,” much like Newton’s name will always be associated with “Newtonian physics.” Yet the hardworking people who test those theories are never going to achieve the same level of recognition. You can call it cruel, but it is simply reality.
Einstein was a creative genius – as are most of the names that have staying power in science. That’s not to say that every creative scientist gets appropriately recognized for their work, but without creativity, there isn’t much possibility of recognition. That’s because we humans deeply value creativity, even though we rarely speak of it. The word creativity is (usually) reserved for artists and musicians and the like.
In my former department, we spent an extraordinary amount of time and energy putting students through the prelim exam process, to assure they were sufficiently qualified to go onto a doctorate. We’d grill and quiz students: do they have the proper control experiments, are the experiments really going to test the hypothesis properly? Will the outcomes be interpretable?
But in all of that work and effort, there was precious little attention paid to helping the students learn to develop better hypotheses and to ask better questions in the first place. Sometimes this would happen incidentally as part of the process, but it was not a direct, stated goal. The process did nothing to vet the students for this type of creativity, though I would argue that its presence or absence will be one of the greatest determinants of the student’s future contributions to the lot of mankind.
I think there’s a simple reason why: hypothesis generation, and asking good questions, involves creative, right-brained thinking. Nobody pays any attention to it, because “creativity” and similar right-brained activities are assumed to have no place in hard science.
Modern scientific training is nearly always focused on left-brain activities like deduction, hypothesis testing, calculation, statistics, and so on.
Yet the big leaps in science come from synthesis, i.e. putting disparate facts together to produce fundamental new leaps in a field. The best scientists – the ones you hear about in the news, who get the big prizes, and so on – all do this. But most aren’t aware that they are doing it, or how vital this ability is to their own success. They are successful, often without knowing why.
And so when it comes to training the next generation, it is all but ignored. It is swept under the rug, like crumbs after a pizza party.
Creativity training, under the guise of more palatable terms like “hypothesis generation” or “how to ask great scientific questions” should be a part of every undergrad and graduate scientific curriculum. And maybe as a side benefit, by exercising the right brain a bit more often, these youngsters would learn how to write more coherently and expressively (since writing is a right-brained activity).
The student that I mentioned at the beginning of the blog post would have been far better off in life if we’d worked with him on asking good questions from the start, rather than just working with him on how to design good experiments to answer those questions. Both are important, yet one was entirely lacking in his training.
But, alas, it may be quite some time before such classes are added to curricula. In the meantime, here are a few simple exercises you can do, a few of which are inspired by Daniel Pink’s book, A Whole New Mind:
1. Laugh and play. These are right-brained activities. Most people I know do not get enough of either one of these. They will give a work-out to your right brain to get your creative muscles in order.
2. Lighten up. This is closely related to number one, but more basic. It seems our whole culture has gotten into a mode of taking itself far too seriously, and everywhere I go in academia, I see people with long, tired faces. I see people stressed about grants, funding, students, committees, and teaching. You can choose to ignore that stuff, if you want to. If you can’t lighten up, it will be very hard to laugh and play.
3. Get involved with music. One National Academy of Sciences member I know likes to play Jazz Piano at night clubs. Most of the brilliant folks I know have a strong affinity for music – listening to it, creating it, or both.
4. Write every day. Reflecting back on my own career, the times that I really struggled to write were the times when I was not doing it regularly, then came up against a deadline and was forced to do it in one big rush. That’s like running a marathon without any training beforehand. Almost nobody would attempt that, and if they did, much pain would result. Yet I see people doing this all the time in academic work, such as when grant proposals are due.
5. Focus on good design. Good designs – whether in art, architecture, or in experiments – are timeless. While there is no quantitative way to measure good design (that I know of), it is usually obvious when you see it. Place a premium on coming up with better experimental designs, on developing better grant proposals. Become emotionally involved in what you’re doing, rather than justturning the crank.
Your right brain needs exercise, because our society – and particularly, academia – doesn’t value what it does. Yet increasingly, it is vital to your success in the world, because it is what drives the important leaps forward in all areas of human progress. We are at a point in history when some big leaps forward are sorely needed.
And, if you would like some insights on the right-brained activity of grant writing, to overcome any challenges you may be having with it, I may be able to help.
|
Bravo Giddings!
this reminds me the essay on how to write boring papers. Brilliant, and I share it completely. My favorite sentence is: “without deviation from the norm, progress is not possible”. Students start to be good when they pose interesting questions, but teachers tend to be satisfied when their students provide clever answers to pre-packaged questions. The only problem I see is that, if Giddings felt the urge to write these things, then the majority of scientists are NOT that way. And probably will not become that way. It is the same old story of breaking the rules against the establishment. It’s the “one billion flies can’t be wrong, s… is good” routine. Those attitudes are sometimes successful, as it happened with Einstein, and sometimes are not, as it happened with John Egler with ecology. Or they are recognized only long after the ideas are originated, as it happened with Mendel. Who happened to be dead when his merits were recognized! The five rules cannot be learnt, you are that way, or you are not. Giddings is right in stressing these qualities, because they should be valued, when present. They are the route to innovation, to deviation from the norm. I guess Giddings operates in the US, though. He should try to write a grant for the European Union!
By the way, if you go in a marine biological station you find many people who are that way, they go in the field, they dive, they are happy. If you go in labs where work is more linked to routines, people tend to be different. My 35 years of experience in marine labs, though, are giving me the impression that these are less happy places than they were before. So, Giddings (or Pink) rules must be enforced even there, because the tendency is in exactly the opposite direction.
I’m reminded of a few things…
a. Doing new things is easy in science. Doing new things that are important in science is much harder.
b. Invention cost money and innovation has the potential to produce money (i.e. value). Far less than 1% of inventions are innovations.
c. Many of the great things come from the unexpected result to the wrong hypothesis. We learn a lot if we let luck do some of the teaching.
I am a principal and was with scientist title during 1999-2001. Just now I opened your site. Soon, I may make a better commment.
I agree with your five points. I love to laugh and play, I like not to take myself too seriously, and though no player myself, I have an incredibly high response to music, the art that touches me most. It is also true that I feel “rusty” writing when I have not been doing some serious writing for some time and I love to do things well for their own sake – whether a grant application, an experimental design or a recipe.
Yet I believe that creative people do not abound currently in science. There are many “scientist-bureaucrats” at all levels that just turn the crank (and that’s OK, they are needed and their work is sometimes sterling quality). There are other “committee-scientists” that just play power games to go to the top, irrespective of their scientific abilities (lots of first-rate social abilities there!). And the current rat race of funding is making people lose a lot of time, leaving little space for creativity.
I am most pleased when one of my students starts asking questions that I cannot answer and showing original ideas. I’d love to have that in all my students, but this is not the case, more often than not. I think that the majority of the scientific people is not highly creative (just think about how many really creative people you meet in congresses and so on). But to be a top scientist you must be creative, for sure.
You are absolutely correct. For far too long the attitude in science was that all ideas had more or less equal merit. In spite of the NIH’s laudable efforts to change the culture of grant review, the old attitudes remain. I saw this very clearly at the last grant review I attended, There was one proposal on which I was reviewer 3. It was beautifully written with very tight experimental design, but there was little reason actually to do the work. The first two reviewers commented mainly on how solid was the approach. My comments were that I did not disagree, but I saw no reason to actually do the work. The contribution to knowledge would be incremental, at best. Interestingly, this same committee had earlier shot down two similar proposals. Different reviewers I suppose.
I believe it was Harriet Zuckerman, the sociologist of science, who examined Einstein’s career in light of two hypotheses. First was that he had the same number of ideas as the rest of us, only his were shifted toward the brilliant end of the distribution of idea merit. The other was that he simply had more ideas that fell on the same distribution, and therefore was able to generate more brilliant ideas because he was also able to sort out the merit of his ideas. By examining his notebooks and writings, she concluded the second hypothesis was closer to the truth than the first. I think far too many scientists sieze on some idea and then seek to follow it until they solve the problem without really examining whether it would matter much in the end if they did. After all, aren’t most journals more interested in publishing solidly done incremental steps than brilliant concepts?
Nicely written. I agree. But would like to add that Creativity maybe valued in the long run, but what is done to reward it in the short term of graduate school? Maybe the reward should be more transparent? The comment made by my cousin springs to mind “We’re scientists not engineers – it isn’t supposed to work.” But it seems that it better-well work if you want to engineer a publication, keep your job, or get a grant based on your “proven expertise”. Personally, the real joy of being a scientists is the following thru on a new idea in a new area. Luckily, I am alive in the great age of computing and can take an exciting idea from 60 to 0 in the time it takes to do a Pubmed/Google search. Imagine the number of ideas that could be examined if one was not forced to ‘produce’ publications. On the flip side would anything ever get published…speaking of which, I need to run a gel. The bands just didn’t look good enough last time…
I went into science because I wanted to use my creativity in that area, and everyone I knew in graduate school was equally or, usually, more creative. Laboratory complements were small; one or two graduate students and zero to two post-doctorates, so the student-mentor interaction was tight. We produced some cool stuff for our professors who were highly passionate about their laboratories’ intellectual efforts. FASEB meetings were exciting, intellectually stimulating events.
Now, that was three decades ago, and I moved off to industry long, long, ago, so I’m wondering if in the intervening years the paperchase for grants and puffy publications sections of c.v.’s, along with the massive availability of full tuition-paying foreign graduate student labor, has degraded the seeking of absolutely truly creative minds that can achieve those monumental research efforts.
I’ll tell you one thing, I still go to Experimental Biology almost every year, and it is amazing how little has come along that astonishes me, that goes beyond my knowledge of pre-1981 physiology. Poster after boring poster, all I can say to myself is, “Yeah, so.” And, forget about questioning any person manning the poster on any topic that would venture outside his or her tiny box of knowledge, because all I get is, “Well, I was assigned this project by my advisor…” So much time, effort, and money for so little outcome and advancement.
Really good scientists are born with creativity, and the first four suggestions by Morgan should have been standard operating life procedures long before they ever got into graduate school. Those students Morgan talks about at the board shouldn’t be sweating, but commanding the room with their intellect. Their brilliant ideas should illuminate the room, and have the strength by themselves to carry the day. Academia needs to go back to basics, and that begins with a total rethink of the students who are accepted into graduate school.
Well said. As an artist, may I also suggest that art exposure in general should help creativity. Going to a good art exhibition will get your “what if” neurons going, no matter what your topic at hand, if you go with an open mind.
On the other hand, I always thought that “too much creativity” might be the downfall of a scientist or of anyone who is trying to achieve a project: what do you do when the ideas keep coming at you and you are bombarded with them. Ideas can be so distracting! So, how about creativity AND discipline?
Very very true. Just please, please, please, if you’re talking about science drop that left/right brain stuff. It’s outdated pseudoscience and it shouldn’t have its place here. Or anywhere else for that matters.
I agree with the conclusions 1-5. However, Einstein never failed math in high school or later on. That’s a common myth; the opposite is true. He mastered advanced calculus, most American students don’t even get to see in college, at the age of 15. He is not a good case for the left-brain, right-brain hypothesis.
The creative process involves more than laughing and playing and appreciating music. While associated with creativity, those activities are not causal; promoting them over a rigorous creative process is like promoting alchemy over physics/chemistry. Unlike the vague techniques you mention, however, there are learned principles and techniques that professional creatives apply in the arts that would equally benefit the sciences. Likewise there are people who are masters at teaching the creative process (my favorite is Robert Fritz at http://www.robertfritz.com) who are able to teach people to understand and apply the creative process in novel areas. As professional artists know, the creative process is rigorous in its own right is able to generate predicable, repeatable successes. Then when their creative work is done, they go laugh and play and hang out at a jazz bar.
This was an excellent article. However, there is no such thing as purely right or left brain thinking. We think in language and right brain operations go immediately to the left side for translation into language and in order to apply rules and thinking to the entire enterprise.
Art may be an exception, depending on the handedness of the visual artist. If he/she wants to underatand or explain what has been done in the right brain, it must first be translated into language to think about it.
Einstein was creative not only with his conceptions, but also in explaining them to others. And, these explanations had an effect on the material in the right visuaospatial side.
Translation into language is returned to the right brain to affect the conception that lays there.
Thus, creativity is a whole brain activity, just like all other intellectual and other kinds of creativity.
Thank you for the excellent article. However, it is to be noted that educators tried to split tasks into righ/left brain differences. This was a failure, because as a model it is deeply flawed as an explanation of thinking and learning.
Please stop propagating the Einstein math myth. He was an excellent student in math and sciences.
http://www.time.com/time/2007/einstein/3.html
http://www.amnh.org/learn/pd/physical_science/profiles/aeinstein.html
This is a rehash of Thomas Kuhn’s famous distinction between the founders of paradigm-shifting scientific theory (like Einstein, Galileo or Darwin) and ordinary puzzle-solving science (like the rest of us who grind out routine applications of pre-existing theoretical insights). What else is there to say? IMHO, not much–except 1) genius is rare and probably cannot be trained at all, and 2) institutionalized science really does encourage linear puzzle-solving.
Can you even teach creativity? I doubt it. The film critic Roger Ebert, in one of his startling insights, writes that the teacher in the movie “Poetry” “…is not a bad teacher, maybe even a good one, although he acts as if you can be taught to write poetry. All you can do is write it. Whether it is good or not isn’t up to you.”
On the other hand, there is lot that we CAN teach students — experimental skills and critical thinking — that will help them leverage their innate creativity. Louis Pasteur put it succinctly: “Fortune favors the prepared mind.” I don’t think he was talking about listening to music.
Very interesting but didn’t Einstein do most/all of his most creative work independant of supervision? Which would seem to indicate he was innately creative? And more importantly had the resilience, self-belief, interest, and drive to complete his theses.
Since every scientist I know would say that creativity is important for science, I don’t see how Dr. Giddings’ assertion that creativity is not valued in science can be correct. But he may be correct that there is insufficient emphasis on creativity in admissons criteria for graduate students and in teaching of science. This situation has probably arisen because creativity is difficult to measure objectively, and even more difficult to teach. Besides, if we only accepted graduate students who we thought were creative, the vast majority of graduate departments would have to close up shop. The vast volumes of data generated by competent-but-not especially-creative scientists are not devoid of value — they serve as fodder for the creative ones to generate new ideas. (Where would Newton have been without the data of Tycho Brahe, or the earlier formulations of Kepler?) (My own personal opinion, not endorsed by NIH or any other agency of the US Government.)
I hate the “I know it when I see it” statement.
Creativity and design are the putting of things together. So, it falls under the “Yet the big leaps in science come from synthesis, i.e. putting disparate facts together to produce fundamental new leaps in a field.”
So, the issue is how do students who have not yet had an opportunity to process the immense amounts of information put such ideas together so that it qualifies as “good design”?
A wise man once said, “the rules of complex organization is my foundation”. So, I encourage exploration into the organizing principles of complexity and apply them, since they will surely be embedded in your model if it is of a natural system.
I had this lingering idea on opening up an art club for faculty and students in my dept. But when I approached a fellow scientists with the idea I was told that I am expected to focus on work alone, and wait until I am tenured to materialize this idea?! The reason to do this was two fold – relieve stress with non competitive art (paint, music, whatever…) and help creativity. To do one good well design experiment, one must ask the question, and to do that one has to “stop thinking so darn hard!”. I benefited from music my entire life as being my one non-science thing that I was free to do whatever in without worry or competition… and at times of hardest deadlines, biggest stress the need to just play would be the greatest and so I would indulge. What would happen after? Well the time “lost” would result in writing whatever science I had to three times faster or simply become more inspired.
I should no share here, but yet I will – I play, simply play with my toddler son every day for a while… build blocks, play trains… and do “nothing” …. then he goes to bed, and I hit the books. The end result, both him and me are happy!
Well, this was what I wanted to share with others…. yet I was told again and again to hide my music, and many other interest until done with what I am supposed to do – get tenured. So why I write this? I agree with above 100%. I also feel relieved to find someone openly talk about this need.
So, my final note – GRADUATE STUDENTS find some time to “waste time” with art, books, movies, dance, music…. and free play in the mud if you must!
ps. for the music check the link: http://www.youtube.com/rockingjulia